|
This has information that is applicable to not only those that are TS but other gender variant states. The language is aimed at the clinician but wade through it and you will find a good bit of information. I do highly recommend the book I have found something new each time I read it. Sandra
Etiology of Gender Dysphoria By
Randi Ettner from Gender Loving Care
Etiology
of Gender Dysphoria
No true
transsexual has yet been persuaded, bullied, drugged, analyzed, shamed,
ridiculed, or electrically shocked into an acceptance of his physique. Jan Morris,
Conundrum
Owing to the intricate relationship of etiology and treatment, it is not surprising that early therapies were designed to cure people of what was considered a psychiatric disorder or, falling short of that goal, to ameliorate their discomfort. Early
hypotheses about the genesis of transsexualism pointed to environmental
factors in the nature vs. nurture antipodal paradigm. The
psychoanalytic model presupposed serious object-relation disturbances and
pathologically introjected, highly cathected motherchild relations (Lothstein,
1979). Those advancing this model maintained that close, retrospective
observations would reveal that the adult transsexual was a child who could not
separate without intense anxiety, on the part of both mother and child (Gilpin,
Raza, & Gilpin, 1979; Moberly, 1986; Ovesey & Person, 1976). The
difficulty lay in the patient's inability to adequately regulate the
intrapsychic distance between self and others (Macvicar, 1978). Psychoanalytic
literature focused on the dynamics beneath the desire to change sex. Various
mechanisms were suggested to account for transsexual wishes. While some
psychoanalytically oriented professionals described the psychopathology to be
of a psychotic nature (Socarides, 1978), others conceptualized a borderline
personality organization. In this theoretical model, the male-to-female
patient seeks to "discard bad and aggressive features and to replace them
with a new, idealized perfection" (Lothstein, 1984). These analysts
suggested that sex reassignment surgery could best be conceptualized as
"a new type of psychosurgery" (Kavanaugh & Volkan, 1978). In the
case of the female-to-male transsexual, the dynamically oriented explanation
was that the need to change sex arose from the child's desire to protect
mother and self from a threatening father (Bradley, 1980). By
1982, the psychoanalytic theory of gender disorders had crystallized into the
following summary position: Sex
reassignment is a symptomatic compromise formation serving defensive and
expressive functions. The symptoms are the outgrowth of developmental trauma
affecting body ego and archaic sense of self caused by peculiar symbiotic and separation-individuation phase relationships. The child exists in the
pathogenic (and reparative) maternal fantasy in order to repair her body image
and to demonstrate the interconvertability of the sexes .... gender
pathology bears common features with other preoedipal syndromes.
Transsexualism is closely linked to perversions and the clinical syndromes may
shade from one into another. However, what is kept at the symbolic level in
the perversions must be made concrete in transsexualism. The clinical
complaint of the transsexual is a condensation of remarkable proportions. When
the transsexual says that he is a girl trapped in a man's body, he sincerely
means what he says. As with other symptoms, however, it takes a long time
before he begins to say what he means. (Meyer, 1982) The
successful treatment of transsexualism via psychoanalysis is claimed in the
literature in scattered case studies (Haber, 1991), with analysis of a
full-blown transference neurosis being viewed as the ultimate resolution (Loeb, 1992; Loeb &
Shane, 1982). A literature review of the last three decades also reports one
case of transsexualism having been cured through exorcism and faith healing!
(Barlow, Abel, & Blanchard, 1977). Throughout
the aforementioned psychiatric literature, one finds frequent mention of the
transsexual's unwillingness to engage in the psychotherapeutic process and
resistance to therapeutic interventions (e.g., Shtasel, 1979). White (1997),
a psychiatrist, has observed similar language invoked in early psychiatric
response to patients who were diagnosed with obsessive-compulsive disorders.
When analysis fails, a "blame the victim" dictum prevails. Running
parallel to the psychoanalytic theories of pathogesesis are the endeavors to
find a biological basis for the condition (Buhrich, Barr,& Lam Po Tang,
1978). Early attempts to find an organic genesis range from roentgenological
examination of the skulls of transsexuals (Lundberg, Sjovall, & Walinder,
1975) to a search for an anomalous hormonal milieu (Gooren, 1986; Kula, Dulko,
Pawlikowski, Imelinski, & Slowikowska, 1986). Some
early studies, first appearing in 1979, looked promising in detecting
incongruous H-Y antigen status in transsexuals (Eicher et al., 1980; Spoljar,
Eicher, Eiermann, & Cleve, 1981). Through cytotoxicity assays, initial
reports found H-Y antigen expression to be discordant with anatomic sex and to
correspond instead to the gender identity of the transsexual (Eicher et al.,
1981; Engel, Pfafflin, & Wiedeking, 1980). HY antigen, thought to be a
presumptive inducer of the testis, is present in the cells of normal males and
not in the cells of normal females. Researchers felt that these results would
"help to replace emotional controversy by rational assessment of
facts" (Vogel, 1981). By
1982, Meyer-Bahlburg concluded that attempts to implicate the H-Y antigen in
the etiology of the condition had failed (Wachtel et al., 1986); nevertheless,
some researchers felt strongly that hormonal-dependent structural brain
changes, though not yet demonstrated, seemed likely (Gooten, 1990). Similarly
failing to bear fruit were studies that documented a high rate of temporal EEG
abnormalities in transsexuals. Despite early accounts of this phenomenon (Hoenig,
& Kenna, 1979; Nusselt & Kockott, 1976), subsequent reports, which
refuted the reliability of visual EEG analyses and utilized quantitative
frequency EEG analysis, found no significant differences between
transsexuals and normals (Grasser, Keidel, & Kockott, 1989 ). And in 1991
a study using magnetic resonance imaging ruled out differences in shape and
size of the splenium of the corpus callosum as a marker of transsexualism
(Emory, Williams, Cole, Amparo, & Meyer). Clinical
psychologists had a unique contribution to make in this search for causality.
Through the use of psychological tests they could substantiate, or fail to
substantiate, the claim that transsexuals had rampant psychopathology. Once
again, findings reported in the literature could buttress either position.
For example, a 1985 study hypothesized that transsexuals manifested a
borderline personality organization and operationalized Kernberg's criteria
for borderline personality with the use of Rorschach protocols. This study
found that, compared to normals, transsexuals and borderlines display intense
levels of aggression, poor object relations, poor reality testing, and
impaired boundary differentiation. The author concluded that male gender
dysphorics are a subgroup of borderline disorders (Murray, 19851. Studies
that looked at sex-change applicants' responses to Minnesota Multiphasic
Personality Inventory testing were evenly divided in concluding that they do
or do not shows signs of, phychopathology (Greenberg & Laurence, 1981).
Some of these differences were clarified when researchers distinguished
between sex-reassignment applicants who were living as men and those who were
living in their preferred female gender. The In a
well-designed research study, Cole, O'Boyte, Emory, and Meyer (1997)
retrospectively examined the comorbidity between gender dysphoria and
psychopathology by reviewing the charts of 435 gender dysphoric
individuals (318 males and 117 females). Extensive information existed for
this group, including MMPI data. They reported an absence of Axis I and Axis
II diagnoses and concluded that transsexualism "is usually an isolated
diagnosis and not part of any general psychopathological disorder." In
other words, transsexuals did not inevitably have a coexisting psychiatric
disorder, which seemed to be the operating assumption of many professionals (Bodlund,
Kullgren, Sundbom, & Hojerback, 1993). In a
discussion of Dutch adolescent transsexuals who had been surgically
reassigned, Cohen Kettenis and Van Goozen (1997) noted that adolescent
transsexuals are psychologically healthier than adult transsexuals. This
suggests that the longer an individual suffers by living unsuccessfully in his
or her phenotypic role, the more vulnerable he or she is to depression ensuing
from the social and emotional difficulties of trying to hide the condition.
Another study of adolescent transsexuals in the Netherlands compared them to
adolescent psychiatric outpatients. These authors concluded that "the
argument that gross psychopatholgy is a required condition for the
development of transsexualism appears indefensible" (Cohen, de Ruiter,
Ringelberg, & Cohen-Kettenis, 1997). As more
and more people came forth requesting treatment and medical resources,
interested investigators began to compile substantial data about the medical
and psychological histories of these individuals. While the histories were
consistent, in that all of these individuals had suffered similar distress,
there was no commonality in terms of environmental features or in any of the
particulars of their biographical data. Some reported unhappy childhoods and
broken homes or blatantly dysfunctional families, while others were born to
privilege or claimed to have had happy childhoods (Buhrich & McConaghy,
1978). Few of the population were sexually abused, as some theorists had
speculated, and there were no similarities in terms of birth order, sexual
experiences in childhood, or childrearing practices of the parents. In
short, there was no empirical evidence that environmental factors alone could
account for the origin of the condition. Interestingly,
the most compelling clues as to the origin of the condition came from a
seemingly unrelated group of people: baby girls who were born with
adrenogenital syndrome. In this syndrome, baby girls are born without an
important enzyme necessary for the making of an adrenal hormone called
cortisol. They have mixed genitalia, which include an oversized clitoris that
resembles a penis. Left untreated, at four years of age their appearance will
begin to show signs of masculinity. Treatment
for this condition became available in the 1950s. It consists of administering
corticosteroids from birth on and surgically correcting the appearance of the
genitals so that they become decidedly female. With this treatment, these
babies are indistinguishable from other infant girls. But as they mature, they
show behavior more typical of boys than girls (Zucker et al., 1996).
Longitudinal studies of these children found them to be tomboys, and as they
grew, they all remarked that they would have preferred to have been male. This
paralleled the transsexual experience and cast further doubt on the nurture
component of the nature vs. nurture controversy. If it
is indeed true that there is a biological etiology of transsexualism, it is
based on the belief that gender identity derives from hormone-induced cephalic
differentiation at some critical gestational stage (Dorner, Poppe, Stahl,
Kolzsch, & Uebelhack, 1991; Elias & Valenta, 1992; Giordano &
Giusti, 1995). A derivative designed to test this belief would be to determine
if any phenotypic differences separate the transsexual population from the
general population. A 1992 report published in the Journal of the American
Medical Association documented that three times as many male-to-female
transsexuals were left-handed, compared to the general population (Watson
& Coren, 1992). In a 1996 study that looked at height as a phenotypic
variable, Ettner, Schacht, Brown, Niederberger, and Schrang demonstrated that
the male transsexual population tends to be of greater than average height. Looking
at pathophysiology of the female-to-male transsexual, Van Straalen, Hage,
and Bloemena (1995) used histological investigation to conclude that an
anomalous inframammary ligament extending from the sternum to the lateral
margin of the pectoralis major muscle is present in female-to-male
transsexuals. Several studies have documented a higher rate of potycystic
ovaries in this population (Bosinski et al., 1997a; Futterweit, 1983;
Futterweit & Deligdisch, 1986; Futterweit & Krieges; 1979; Futterweit,
Weiss, & Fagerstrom, 1986; Spinder, Spijkstra, Gooren, & Burger,
1989). Bosinski
et al. (1997b) examined the relationship between body build, androgens, and
female-to-male transsexualism. They assessed anthropometrical measurements in
hormonally untreated female-to-males in comparison to healthy female and male
controls. The female-to-male group differed from control females on seven of
fourteen sex-dimorphic indices of masculinity/femininity in body build. The
transsexuals were more masculine in body shape, primarily in bone proportion
and fat distribution. Levels of testosterone and androstenedione were significantly
higher in the female-to-males than in the control females. Unbound
testosterone was also higher in female-to-males than in control females, and
correlated positively with masculine body configuration. White
has theorized that "neuronal pruning" may be the process responsible
for normal gender identity development as well as transsexualism--a common
filial pathway encompassing varying degrees of nature and nurture. It is
assumed that gender identity is a process that begins in-utero and is
consolidated by three years of age. White suggests that the fetal brain is
primed for gender identity development by an anatomic "sculpting" of
location. This location is impacted by an active pruning of neurons during the
third trimester of fetal life. The mechanisms involved in gender identity
consolidation may follow patterns similar to those of other developmental
processes, such as language acquisition: The neural
proliferation within the neural tube undergoes a caudal migration ending
between weeks 18 to 24. Over half of the neurons and glial cells generated
undergo an active pruning process--a programmed cell death known as apoptosis.
Genes become activated to prune specific brain cells and once pruned they
cannot regenerate. Activation of these cellular "suicide" genes
involves genetic, hormonal, and immunologic factors, although chance may also
play a role. It has
been shown that the right hemisphere develops approximately ten days before
the left. In most individuals, there is an active pruning of the right
hemisphere such that when the left hemisphere develops it claims dominance.
If this active pruning on the right does not occur; and if this involves the
motor strip of the brain, the right hemisphere may attain dominance resulting
in a lefthanded individual. If this process involves the language center,
the result may be dyslexia. Since testosterone is involved in cell survival,
it potentially can effect pruning and thus may account for the increased rates
of dyslexia in boys. If this same process involves the area in the brain
wherein lies one's sense of gender identity (one promising possibility is the
central subdivision of the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis [BSTc] in the
hypothalamus noted by Zhou et al., 1995, to be a sexually dimorphic
nucleus),'then it could potentially result in a discordant gender identity. In
some manner that is poorly understood, within the 'structural components of
the brain, within these connections of neurons and synapses, lies one's core
sense of gender self. As with any developmental process, the route taken may
be quite different than expected. Neurons, too, can take the "road less
traveled." Yet
neurons are only one piece of the equation and the potential etiologic factors
span from neuronal migration to synaptic pruning, the latter being mainly a
postnatal phenomenon. Differentiation of the primary sexual features occurs
during approximately the third month of uterine life. At this stage of
development, the neurons are undergoing a rapid proliferation and migration.
Testoterone not only serves as the key ingredient propagating primary
sexual development in the normal male but may also differentiate brain
development through modulation of either the migration or pruning process.
Whether via a receptor defect, inadequate hormonal levels, or genetic
abnormalities of migration or pruning, the brain region responsible for gender
identity may be either predisposed or set to a specific gender identity. In
light of the developmental processes at this time, this period is certainly
worthy of consideration in exploring the etiologic cascade. If
gender identity consolidation relies heavily on nurture, then synaptic
pruning within an epigenetic framework is an etiologic candidate.
Synaptogenesis, or the creation of synapses, begins during the second
trimester and is most rapid during the first year of life, although this is
somewhat dependent on the region of the brain. As with neurons, the synapses
also undergo an active pruning process that correlates with a decline in brain
plasticity. Since synaptic activity accounts for approximately fifty percent
of this pruning process, it is feasible that certain environmental triggers
could augment a discordant gender identity--most probably in the context of an
underlying predisposition. Nurture can modulate nature through mechanisms such
as immediate-early genes. Whichever pathway, be it migration, neuronal
genesis, or pruning and/or synaptogenesis or pruning, gender identity becomes
fixed over time with the decrement of brain plasticity. Once crystallized,
it becomes as difficult to change as forgetting one's mother tongue. (1997) With
sophisticated new technologies, such as functional magnetic resonance
imaging and positron emission tomography, brain activity and structure can be
envisaged as never before. At present, there is a rapidly mounting assemblage
of brain structure research. These studies reveal differences in the
structure and function of female and male brains. Zhou, Hofman, Gooren, and
Swaab at the Netherlands Institute for Brain Research in Amsterdam broke new
ground in announcing, in 1995, that there exists a detectable difference in
transsexual brains, as viewed at autopsy. The
specific region under study is the area of the bed nucleus of the stria
terminals, BSTc, an area essential for sexual behavior. The autopsied brains
revealed that the genetically male transsexuals had a female brain structure,
for the volume of the central sulcus of the stria terminalis of the
hypothalamus was similar in male-to-female transsexuals and genetic females.
Heterosexual and homosexual males displayed a larger volume. This supports the
hypothesis that gender identity develops as an interactive affair between the
brain and sex hormones. This
research has had an enormous impact. Some European laws have been changed to
reflect this new recognition that transsexuals are born, not made, and as such
deserve medical care and legal protection. We in the United States have not
followed suit, as yet. An
unfortunate surgical accident and its widely reported consequences seemed to
buttress this view of gender identity as existing independent of gonadal and anatomical sex. In 1973,
researchers at Johns Hopkins University published the case of an infant male
twin whose penis was injured, and then amputated. The child was reared as a
girl, and the case seemed to offer proof that infants are genderless at birth
and that the process of socialization establishes gender identity. But
in 1997, Milton Diamond and H. Keith Sigmundson presented a long term followup of this highly publicized case that sharply contradicted the alleged
success of this child's reassignment to a female gender. The patient, who
was raised as a girl, knew nothing of the accident that occurred at birth, and
though given female hormones, steadfastly maintained a male identity.
"Joan" as the infant was named, would tear off her dresses anti
reject dolls, and consistently preferred male companions. She rebuked her
mother's attempts to have her wear makeup and tried to urinate standing. As
she entered puberty and grew breasts, she stopped taking the female hormones
because she so disliked the changes that were occurring. At that point she was
suicidal. At age 14, still unaware of her past, she refused to continue life
as a girl. Her father broke down and revealed the surgical accident, the
subsequent removal of the testes, and the creation of the neovagina. This
information proved to be a great relief to Joan. For the first time, life made
sense. Joan requested male hormones, had mastectomy, and began phalloplasty
to try to regain a male anatomy. At 25, "John" married. Even though
the reassignment surgeries were only moderately successful, John was happier
than ever before. In a
second documented case of a boy similarly reared as a girl after the genitals
were damaged during circumcision, the outcome was unlike that described above.
In this newly publicized case, the reassigned child adapted to a female
identity, lives as a woman, and describes herself as bisexual
("Manipulating Gender," 1998). In the first case the reassignment
took place at twenty-one months, whereas in the second case surgery was
performed at seven months. These cases suggest that, while gender identity
is not solely a result of socialization, there appears to be a critical
period--a window of time after birth--during which gender identity
consolidates.
|
Send mail to The Gender Tree with
questions or comments about this web site.
|